Researchers begin to unravel puzzle of language learning in children
A unique has thrown fascinating new light on how young children begin to understand the meaning of words.
The researchers, from The University of Manchester publish their findings in the journal Child Development.
Children start to say words around their first birthday, and for a while only say one word at a time, though they rapidly build their vocabulary during their second year.
But the researchers found they do not do this by adding a complete form of new words to their own personal dictionary.
Instead, they put a new word in their dictionary which has some, but not all of the meaning, slowly finetuning it as they hear more language.
To show how children do this, the researchers set up a study in Manchester Museum, working with a group of three to eight-year olds.
An experimenter built either 4 blocks stacked up, or 4 blocks lined up flat on a table, and then the children were asked to respond to different size words by building a bigger, smaller or taller version.
The researchers compared how their structure differed from the experimenter青瓜视频檚 in each dimension , using mathematical modelling to describe what types of changes children made, and how patterns varied with age.
Three and four-year-olds tended to treat bigger, smaller, and taller with the same meaning: they built things that were bigger in all directions.
青瓜视频淚t seems that when children first learn words, they pick up a general idea of what they mean- in this case, that the words mean a size change青瓜视频, said co- author Dr Alissa Ferry, a lecturer at The University of Manchester.
青瓜视频淭his seems to be how we end up with children calling a cow a dog, or all round fruit apples, even though they青瓜视频檝e never heard an adult do that. But with more experience they fine tune their word meanings.
青瓜视频淲e do think all children go through this process of fine-tuning word meanings, but which words are fine-tuned and when depends on what they hear around them.青瓜视频
It seems that when children first learn words, they pick up a general idea of what they mean- in this case, that the words mean a size change. This seems to be how we end up with children calling a cow a dog, or all round fruit apples, even though they青瓜视频檝e never heard an adult do that. But with more experience they fine tune their word meanings
青瓜视频淪ize words青瓜视频, explained Co Author Dr Katherine Twomey, also from the University of Manchester, 青瓜视频渁re trickier to learn because they describe relations between all different kinds of objects, which makes it harder to find what is common.
青瓜视频淭hat makes it easier for us to see how the meaning changes with age development.青瓜视频
By around age 5, the children generally worked out that smaller meant they should use fewer blocks.
But it was not until about age seven they reliably worked out that taller really means bigger but specifically in the 青瓜视频榰p青瓜视频 direction.
Most of the 3-year-olds built bigger things when the researchers asked for smaller ones, though some of them seemed to work it out faster than others.
It was not until about age 7 when most of the children knew that taller meant specifically 青瓜视频榰p青瓜视频.
However some 3- and 4-year-olds already seemed to know that taller meant 青瓜视频榰p青瓜视频, probably because they had exposure to those words more frequently in conversations with their caregivers.
Dr Ferry added: 青瓜视频淟earning a language is a uniquely human experience; children just pick it up from being exposed to it.
青瓜视频淵et, we don青瓜视频檛 quite know how that happens, which is why we carried out this study.青瓜视频
Also on the research team were four sixth form Nuffield Research Placement summer internship students who helped design and collect the data.
The paper Bigger Versus Smaller: Children's Understanding of Size Comparison Words Becomes More Precise With Age is published